Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's Archives of Pharmacology
© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014
10.1007/s00210-013-0955-z

Differential effects of paradoxical sleep deprivation on memory and oxidative stress

Alisson Menezes Araujo Lima1, Veralice Meireles Sales de Bruin , Emiliano Ricardo Vasconcelos Rios2 and Pedro Felipe Carvalhedo de Bruin1
(1)
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciências Médicas, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade Federal do Ceará, R. Cel Nunes de Melo 1315, Rodolfo Teófilo, 60.430-270 Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
(2)
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Farmacologia, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Brazil
 
 
Veralice Meireles Sales de Bruin
Received: 11 July 2013Accepted: 25 December 2013Published online: 15 January 2014
Abstract
Sleep has important functions for every organ in the body and sleep deprivation (SD) leads to disorders that cause irreparable damage. The aim of this study was to investigate behavioral and brain structural alterations in mice deprived of paradoxical sleep for 48 and 72 h. Working memory, aversive memory as well as levels of nitric oxide (NO) and thiobarbituric acid reactive substances (TBARS) in the hippocampus, body striatum, and prefrontal cortex were evaluated. Working memory was affected in the 48- and 72-h SD groups while aversive memory was altered only in the 48-h SD group (p ≤ 0.05). Our findings showed that SD reduces NO levels in most brain areas (p  < 0.05): NO levels were unaltered in the striatum of animals sleep-deprived for 48 h. Higher levels of TBARS were observed in all areas of the SD groups (p ≤  0.05). Thus, we confirmed that SD has duration-dependent effects on behavior as well as on NO and TBARS levels in the brain. Preserved striatum NO levels suggest that this structure is less vulnerable to oxidative stress and is only affected by SD of longer duration. Increased TBARS and reduced NO levels in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex confirm a central role for both these structures in working memory and aversive memory. Contextual fear conditioning was not affected by longer periods of SD. Thus, our findings suggest that shorter SD time may be more beneficial to avoid aversive memory where this may have implications for the management of posttraumatic stress.
Keywords
Paradoxical sleep deprivation Oxidative stress Nitric oxide TBARS Aversive memory Striatum

Introduction

Sleep is essential for the consolidation of memory besides numerous other health-related functions. This is a complex process: evidence shows that sleep influences memory consolidation depending on type of memory, psychological conditions, and expected reward gain, among other factors (Diekelmann et al. 2009). Specific circuitry may possibly be involved and it has been hypothesized that prefrontal-hippocampal circuitry is essential for memory consolidation during sleep (Diekelmann et al. 2009; Varela et al. 2013). Recently, it has been suggested that reuniens cells located in the thalamus directly coordinate activity in medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus and are important for memory consolidation (Varela et al. 2013).
It has been previously demonstrated that sleep deprivation increases oxidative stress in the brain (Vollert et al. 2011; Alzoubi et al. 2012). Several studies have confirmed that the effects of sleep deprivation are at least partially mediated by reactive oxygen species (ROS) (Ramanathan et al. 2002; Brown and Naidoo 2010). In fact, sleep deprivation induces noxious metabolic and immunological alterations that eventually lead to lethal consequences and it is thought that antioxidant imbalance mediates these alterations (Everson et al. 2005). A theory that sleep removes free radicals accumulated in the brain during wakefulness has been proposed (Reimund 1994). Understanding how memory and different parts of the brain are affected by oxidative stress following sleep deprivation is a subject that warrants further elucidation.
Evidence suggests that an interruption in the nitric oxide (NO)/cyclic guanosine monophosphate (cGMP) pathway signaling dopamine can decrease the regulation of the NO/cGMP/CREB (Thatcher et al. 2006). This latter binding protein is essential for the cAMP response involved in the process of learning and memory. Thus, a direct connection between NO and learning/memory function exists. Given that free radicals, which include reactive nitrogen and reactive oxygen species, are hard to measure because of their extreme reactivity, NO levels can be secondarily deduced from levels of its metabolites, nitrates/nitrites. Notably, NO is the only endogenous source of brain nitrates and nitrites, because dietary nitrates and nitrites do not cross the blood–brain barrier (Ramanathan et al. 2002). Nitrite is a stable compound that has been frequently used as a marker of NO production; however, it only reflects the summation of the events occurring for nitric oxide in a given time window (Clement et al. 2004). When antioxidants are incapable of scavenging the free radicals, lipids will suffer oxidative damage and express thiobarbituric acid reactive substances (TBARS) (Ramanathan and Siegel 2011).
Some studies have reported the role of NO in regulating the sleep-wake cycle, probably by homeostatic processes (Kapas et al. 1994; Dzoljic et al. 1997; Burlet et al. 1999; Clement et al. 2004). Previously, a subset of cortical neurons, GABAergic interneurons, that express neuronal NO synthase have been related to slow-wave activity (Gerashchenko et al. 2008). Cortical neurons immunoreactive for neuronal NO synthase and the neurokinin-1 receptor have been proposed to be involved in this physiological process (Wisor et al. 2011; Morairty et al. 2013). Moreover, sleep deprivation has been shown to increase the expression of NO synthase in prolonged wakefulness-active neurons labeled by Fos. The increase of NO synthase expression in wakefulness-active neurons has been positively correlated with sleep pressure, suggesting a role for NO-producing cells in the homeostatic control of sleep (Kalinchuk et al. 2010).
Clinical and/or biochemical factors, such as nutrition (Vazquez-Medina et al. 2012), physical activity (Nielsen et al. 2007), neurotransmitter abnormalities (Das et al. 2008), and cytoskeletal protein damage (Rodriguez-Vazquez et al. 2012) have been implicated in the genesis of neuronal damage (Zhang et al. 2010). In fact, the response or adaptation to sleep deprivation is a complex process under multifactorial influences including the use of medications that affect neurotransmission (de Oliveira et al. 2004). The duration of sleep deprivation is one of the factors that may induce differential cerebral alterations. For instance, a study has shown that seals use an adaptive mechanism of hormetic response to intermittent hypoxia and increased oxidative stress (Vazquez-Medina et al. 2012). Moreover, time-specific effects of sleep deprivation on cytoskeletal protein damage have been shown (Rodriguez-Vazquez et al. 2012). Considering this body of evidence and the possibility that adaptive response to oxidative stress may occur, it follows that the duration of paradoxical sleep deprivation may differentially affect memory and oxidative stress in the various parts of the brain. The aim of this study was to evaluate memory function and levels of nitrite and TBARS as indicators of oxidative stress and lipid oxidative damage in various different brain areas in mice subjected to paradoxical sleep deprivation for 48 and 72 h.

Materials and methods

Animals

Adult Swiss albino male mice, weighing between 25 and 30 g, were used. Animals were kept in plastic cages, in groups of five, lined with wood shavings; exposed to a natural 24-h light/dark cycle; and fed standard chow and water ad libitum. The study followed the ethical principles of animal experimentation established by the Brazilian College of Animal Experimentation (COBEA) and was previously approved by the Ethics Committee in Animal Research (CEPA-67-09).

Paradoxical sleep deprivation

The methods used to induce REM deprivation was based on the multiple platform method adapted for mice. van Hulzen and Coenen (1981) introduced the multiple platform method using several platforms in a large water tank, reducing the movement restriction stress induced by the single platform technique (van Hulzen and Coenen 1981). In the present study, the multiple platform method was further modified. Five platforms (5 cm in diameter), placed in a tank (40 × 30 cm) filled with water to within 1 cm of the upper surface of the platforms, were used. Five mice, taken from the same cage in which they were previously housed, were placed in each tank for 48 or 72 h, with water and food ad libitum. The method consists of placing the animals in a compartment with several narrow platforms surrounded by water. Once accustomed to these conditions, the animals can achieve slow-wave sleep, whereas REM sleep is restricted. The loss of muscle tone associated with sleep deprivation results in animals touching the water and awakening. This multiple platform model does not impose restriction of movement or social isolation (Patti et al. 2010).

Experimental protocol

Experimental procedure animals were distributed into three groups (N  = 8 each). Mice were submitted to 48- or 72-h paradoxical sleep deprivation or kept in their home cages for control. Behavioral tests were performed in all groups and after paradoxical sleep deprivation in the 48- and 72-h groups. Animals were decapitated immediately after behavioral tests (15 min), and the striatum, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex were then dissected and frozen for analysis. All behavioral tests, the Y-maze (8 min) and step-down (5 min), were performed immediately after the end of sleep deprivation in the 48- and 72-h groups. The tests were conducted during daytime, under conditions of darkness, from 8:00 am to 9:00 am. Immediately after the behavioral tests (13 to 15 min), animals were sacrificed and brain tissue was collected.

Behavioral tests

Assessment of working memory—Y-maze test

Working memory was assessed by the Y-maze test. In this test, the animals are placed in a Y-shaped maze with three arms of equal length (Sarter et al. 1988). Typically, the animals have a strong tendency to switch entries in different environments. For the evaluation of memory, arms are numbered. Initially, the animal is placed in the apparatus for 8 min and the number of entries into each arm is noted. This is considered a settling time during which animals enter three different arms without repetition. The results are expressed as percentage and obtained through a mathematical formula (Hidaka et al. 2011). The success of the test is indicated by the high switching rate in the control groups, indicating that the animals cannot remember which arm they entered last. Between each session, the maze is cleaned with a 20 % alcohol solution and dried using paper towels.

Evaluation of aversive memory—step-down test

One way to assess the conditioned response to fear is through the passive avoidance test (DeNoble et al. 1986). During the test, inhibition of the animal’s natural tendency to descend onto the lower platform due to the presence of an aversive component constitutes a conditioned response. For this test, animals were previously trained. They were placed in the portable housing for the new environment (1 min), training (5 min), and subsequent test (15 min, evaluating short-term memory) procedures. For the analysis of delayed recall, the animals were subjected to the test (5 min) again after 48 and 72 h (with and without sleep deprivation).

Nitrite levels in the sample

This method is based on the use of the Griess reagent which detects the presence of nitrite in the sample through a diazotization reaction by formation of a chromophore pink color. The reagent is prepared using equal parts of 5 % phosphoric acid, N-1-naphthalenediamine (NEED) 0.1 %, 1 % sulfanilamide in 5 % phosphoric acid, and distilled water. The assay was performed by the addition of 100 μL of the supernatant of the homogenate at 10 %, comprising potassium phosphate buffer in 100 μL of Griess reagent, to the white 100 μL in 100 μL of reagent buffer. The standard curve was obtained through serial dilutions (100, 50, 25, 12.5, 6.25, 3.12, 1.56 μM) of nitrite. The entire assay was performed in a 96-well plate and absorbance readings taken in the 560-nm range (Patti et al. 2010).

Determination of lipid peroxidation

The rate of lipid peroxidation was estimated by determination of malondialdehyde (MDA) using the TBARS test. The samples were washed with cold saline to minimize interference of hemoglobin with free radicals. All samples were homogenized at 10 % with potassium phosphate buffer, withdrawn at a rate of 250 μL and placed in a water bath at 37 °°C for 1 h. Subsequently, 400 μL of 35 % perchloric acid was added and centrifuged at 14,000 rpm for 20 min at 4 °C. The supernatant was removed and placed in contact with 400 μL of 0.6 % thiobarbituric acid. The resultant mixture was then incubated at 95–100 °C for 1 h. After cooling, the mixture absorbance of the supernatant was read at 532 nm. A standard curve was generated using 1,1,3,3-tetramethoxypropane. Results were expressed as nanomoles of MDA per milligram of protein (Khadrawy et al. 2011).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics are presented as mean (±standard deviation), range, and frequency (percentage values). All data were tested for normal distribution and equal variance. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post hoc test or Student’s t test was employed to compare results of oxidative stress biomarkers between multiple groups and between sleep-deprived animals versus controls, respectively. The Kruskall-Wallis test and Mann-Whitney U test for continuous variables were used to compare behavioral test results between multiple groups and between sleep-deprived animals versus controls, respectively. Statistical analysis was carried out using SPSS for Windows, version 16.0. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Our findings show that sleep deprivation reduced NO levels in almost all regions studied (with the exception of the striatum in animals deprived of sleep for 48 h) and increased TBARS levels in all areas and groups. Working memory was affected in both of the sleep-deprived groups while aversive memory was affected only in the group that was sleep-deprived for 48 h.
Animal groups sleep-deprived for 48 (p = 0.002) and 72 h (p = 0.000) showed worse performance on the Y-maze test (Fig. 1). Animals sleep-deprived for 48 h showed impaired aversive memory as evaluated by the step-down test. Animals sleep-deprived for 72 h showed no deficit in aversive memory compared to controls (Fig. 2).
/static-content/images/394/art%253A10.1007%252Fs00210-013-0955-z/MediaObjects/210_2013_955_Fig1_HTML.gif
Fig. 1
Effect of paradoxical sleep deprivation on Y-maze test performance in mice. Compared to controls, both groups of mice sleep-deprived for 48 (N = 8) and 72 h (N = 8) showed worse performance in working memory as evaluated by the Y-maze test (a,bMann-Whitney test, p < 0.05). The Mann-Whitney test was used for comparison between control and 48 h sleep-deprived groups (a p = 0.002) and between control and 72-h sleep-deprived groups (b p = 0.001)
/static-content/images/394/art%253A10.1007%252Fs00210-013-0955-z/MediaObjects/210_2013_955_Fig2_HTML.gif
Fig. 2
Effect of paradoxical sleep deprivation on step-down test performance in mice. Animals sleep-deprived for 48 h showed worse aversive memory performance as evaluated by step-down in comparison to controls (Mann-Whitney test, p = 0.007). Mice sleep-deprived for 72 h showed no deficit in aversive memory compared to controls

Oxidative stress biomarkers

Nitrite titration

Mice submitted to paradoxical sleep-deprived for 72 h showed higher nitrite levels in the striatum (Fig. 3a, p  = 0.004). No significant difference was found in animals sleep-deprived for 48 h. Both groups of animals sleep-deprived for 48 and 72 h showed higher nitrite levels in the hippocampus (Fig. 3b, p = 0.01 and p  = 0.007, respectively). Higher nitrite values in the prefrontal cortex were also observed in both these groups of animals (Fig. 3c, p = 0.01 and p  = 0.002, respectively). In the striatum, control animals had higher values (123.8 μM, standard error (SE) 6.125) compared to animals deprived of sleep for 48 (92.13 μM, SE 15.73) and 72 h (64.17 μM, SE 5.247). For the hippocampus, control animals showed higher levels (96.67 μM, SE 4.592) compared to animals deprived of sleep for 48 (65.35 μM, SE 5.542) and 72 h (64.88 μM, SE 4.367). In the prefrontal cortex, control animals showed higher values (117.1 μM, SE 6.196) compared to animals sleep-deprived for 48 (58.58 μM, SE 6.508) and 72 h (65.01 μM, SE 3.729).
/static-content/images/394/art%253A10.1007%252Fs00210-013-0955-z/MediaObjects/210_2013_955_Fig3_HTML.gif
Fig. 3
Effects of paradoxical sleep deprivation on oxidative stress asmeasured by nitrite levels in striatum (a), hippocampus (b), and prefrontalcortex (c) of mice. Controls exhibited higher nitrite values in all areascompared to both 48- and 72-h sleep-deprived mice. Results areexpressed considering p<0.05 and 95 % CI (ANOVA followed byBonferroni post hoc test)

TBARS titration

Paradoxical sleep-deprived mice, for both 48 and 72 h, showed significantly lower levels of TBARS in the striatum, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex as demonstrated by Fig. 4. In the striatum, controls (11.74 μmol, SE 2.285 SE) showed lower levels compared to 48- (50.17 μmol, SE 3.454) and 72-h (33.65 μM, SE 2.792) sleep-deprived animals. In the hippocampus, controls showed lower values (6.955 μmol, SE 0.687) compared to 48- (40.96 μmol, SE 3.548) and 72-h (38.34 μM, SE 4.250) sleep-deprived mice. For the prefrontal cortex, controls showed lower TBARS values (7.183 μmol, SE 0.988) compared to 48- (37.22 μmol, SE 3.921) and 72-h (39.83 μM, SE 2.033) sleep-deprived mice. Mean and standard deviation values obtained in this study are shown in Table 1.
/static-content/images/394/art%253A10.1007%252Fs00210-013-0955-z/MediaObjects/210_2013_955_Fig4_HTML.gif
Fig. 4
Effect of paradoxical sleep deprivation on oxidative stress as measured by TBARS levels in striatum body (d), hippocampus (e), and prefrontal cortex (f) of mice. Controls exhibited lower TBARS levels in the striatum compared to both 48- and 72-h sleep-deprived mice (ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test, p  < 0.05). Controls exhibited lower TBARS levels in the hippocampus compared to both 48- and 72-h sleep-deprived mice (ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test, p  < 0.05). Controls exhibited lower TBARS levels in the prefrontal cortex compared to both 48- and 72-h sleep-deprived mice (ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test, p < 0.05)
Table 1
Descriptive analysis of memory tests (Y-maze and step-down) and biochemical analyses for NO and TBARS in all groups
 
Control
48 h PSD
72 h PSD
Y-maze
80.83 % (±1.98 SE)
57.51 % (±3.22 SE)
66.40 % (±2.72 SE)
Step-down
276.4 s (±23.60; 48 h)
71.80 s (±26.76 SE)
300.0 s (±0)
272.0 s (±23.32; 72 h)
Nitrite SB
123.8 μM (±6.125 SE)
92.13 μM (15.73 ± SE)
64.17 μM (±5.247 SE)
Nitrite HC
96.67 μM (±4.592 SE)
65.35 μM (5.542 ± SE)
64.88 μM (±4.367 SE)
Nitrite PFC
117.1 μM (±6.196 SE)
58.58 μM (6.508 ± SE)
65.01 μM (±3.729 SE)
TBARS SB
11.74 μmol (±2.285 SE)
50.17 μmol (3.454 ± SE)
33.65 μM (±2.792 SE)
TBARS HC
6.955 μmol (±0.687 SE)
40.96 μmol (3.548 ± SE)
38.34 μM (±4.250 SE)
TBARS PFC
7.183 μmol (±0.988 SE)
37.22 μmol (3.921 ± SE)
39.83 μM (±2.033 SE)
PSD paradoxical sleep deprivation, SB striatum body, HC hippocampus, PFC prefrontal cortex, SE standard error

Discussion

Our findings confirm the differential effects of paradoxical sleep deprivation on behavior and oxidative stress in the brain. First, we showed that 48-h paradoxical sleep deprivation reduced posttraining contextual fear conditioning. However, contextual fear conditioning was not affected after longer periods of paradoxical sleep deprivation. These findings suggest that a shorter paradoxical sleep deprivation time is more beneficial for avoiding fear conditioning or posttraumatic stress memory. A previous study indicated that sleep deprivation reduces the development of posttraumatic stress disorder via extinction of the fear-magnifying effects of memory (Kuriyama et al. 2010). In agreement with our findings, Graves et al. (2003) showed that sleep deprivation shortly after training reduced contextual fear conditioning while sleep deprivation after a longer period of training had no effect (Graves et al. 2003). The present findings are also in partial agreement with a previous report indicating that, unlike longer periods, shorter periods of sleep deprivation are associated with disruption of memory reconsolidation (Shi et al. 2011). Our study indicated that a delay in recovery for aversive memory occurs after longer periods of sleep deprivation.
It has been suggested that the biochemical pathways linked to memory consolidation after sleep, possibly related to protein kinase A and protein synthesis inhibitors, are vulnerable to the duration of sleep deprivation (Hellman et al. 2010). Moreover, it has been demonstrated that a combination of sleep deprivation with a Western diet elevates TBARS levels and increases oxidative stress in the hippocampus, thus inducing memory impairment. These factors, when combined, were reported to produce greater impairment than each factor alone (Alzoubi et al. 2013). All these findings show that multiple factors affect memory impairment after sleep deprivation.
In this study, nitrite levels were reduced in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in both the 48- and 72-h sleep-deprived groups. In the striatum, nitrite was reduced only in the 72-h sleep-deprived group. The preservation of striatum nitrite levels suggests that this structure is less vulnerable to oxidative stress and is only affected by a longer period of paradoxical sleep deprivation. Increased TBARS and reduced nitrite levels in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex confirm a central role for these two structures in working memory and aversive memory. Also, in this experimental model, nitrite levels in the striatum did not play a key role for working and aversive memory. Lipid peroxidation, based on analysis of end products such as lipid peroxides, malondialdehydes, and other low molecular weight aldehydes (TBARS), was increased in all 48- and 72-h paradoxical sleep-deprived animals and in all the studied areas including the striatum, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex.
Considering that paradoxical sleep deprivation has been shown to have both harmful and beneficial effects upon several biological functions, we will further discuss the present and previous findings relating to sleep deprivation, memory, and oxidative stress. Controversy remains over the effects of sleep deprivation on oxidative stress products and antioxidants. For instance, a previous study has shown that long periods of sleep deprivation reduce SOD activity in the hippocampus and brainstem while the neocortex showed no changes in association with either short-term or long-term sleep deprivation. It has also been shown that 6 h sleep deprivation increases antioxidants in the hippocampus, cerebellum, and neocortex (Ramanathan et al. 2010). Conversely, 96 h of sleep deprivation decreased antioxidants in the hypothalamus, again, suggesting a delay in recovery from oxidative stress after long periods of sleep deprivation (D’Almeida et al. 1998). In our study, the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex both showed increased TBARS values confirming that these structures are affected by sleep deprivation and are important for memory. The reorganization of human cerebral activity in hippocampus-neocortical networks after sleep has recently been evaluated by functional magnetic resonance imaging (Albouy et al. 2013). The interesting study shows that sleep deprivation does not enhance but stabilizes sleep-dependent motor sequence memory consolidation. Sleepers showed, after retest, increased responses in the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex while sleep-deprived subjects showed increased responses in the putamen and cingulate cortex. Interestingly, it has also been shown that the amygdala is critically involved in contextual fear memory and this is also related to the cholinergic and histaminergic systems (Bucherelli et al. 2006). This is all important considering that motor sequence memory consolidation is physiologically essential. Notably, chronic insomnia has been recently associated with comorbidities and degenerative disorders (Bruin et al. 2012; Miyata et al. 2013). Also, animal experimentation has shown that the parkinsonian mouse model presents increased levels of nitrite in the striatum secondary to chronic exposure to paraquat (Yadav et al. 2013). Our data indicates that the striatum, an important structure implicated in both motor and memory function, is only affected by longer periods of sleep deprivation.
An important aspect is to understand how sleep deprivation increases oxidative stress in the brain. Oxidative stress has been defined as a disturbance in the equilibrium of prooxidants and antioxidants in favor of the former, leading to potentially deleterious effects for lipids, proteins, DNA, and RNA. These reactions can include fatty acid peroxidation of cellular membranes, oxidation of sulfhydryl groups, and enzyme inactivation (Brown and Naidoo 2010). The brain is highly susceptible to oxidative stress due to several factors, namely, high levels of polyunsaturated fatty acids, increased metabolic activity, low antioxidant capacity, and the inability to repair neuronal cells. If the antioxidants are not able to successfully clean radicals, this will lead to oxidative damage to lipids resulting in oxidative stress (Lobo et al. 2010).
Khadrawy et al. (2011) demonstrated that 72 h of REM sleep deprivation increases the production of NO in the rodent hippocampus. In the study, the authors also reported that 72 h of REM sleep deprivation increased lipid oxidation in both areas of the hippocampus and cortex (Khadrawy et al. 2011). Suer et al. (2011) showed that 21 days of intermittent REM sleep deprivation (recovery with 6 h of sleep each day) increased lipid oxidation and decreased activity of SOD and GPx in rat hippocampus (Suer et al. 2011). Silva et al. (2004) demonstrated that 72 h of sleep deprivation increased hippocampal oxidized/reduced glutathione ratio as well as lipid peroxidation, confirming an important role of hippocampal oxidative stress in memory deficits (Silva et al. 2004). Importantly, research has shown that an increase in NO release occurs 18 h after cessation of paradoxical sleep deprivation while a reduction occurs 24 h after cessation of paradoxical sleep deprivation (Clement et al. 2004).
The data presented in our study show that lipid peroxidation (TBARS) increased in all sleep-deprived groups and study areas. Notably, antioxidant activity, as measured by nitrite levels, can modulate this parameter to promote a new redox state of cells and combat the deleterious effects caused by ROS with the possibility of reversing the stress oxidative process. Even with this control, at some point after sleep deprivation, recovery periods may prove inadequate, leading to elevated ROS and failure of the antioxidant defense system to modulate this oxidative process. Thus, an increase in TBARS levels, characterizing a situation of oxidative stress, is observed.
Limitations of this study should be acknowledged. Some antioxidants and protein synthesis inhibitors such as protein kinase A were not measured where this data could have helped clarify the process of sleep deprivation and memory. However, we confirmed differential effects of sleep deprivation on fear conditioned memory and oxidative stress, findings which may be of interest particularly for posttraumatic stress disorder. Also, the multiple platform method used in the present study is associated with less anxiety than the single platform method. Nevertheless, it should be noted that increased psychological stress and muscle fatigue still occur under the multiple system and these should not be overlooked when interpreting our data.

Conclusions

In the present study, we showed that a shorter paradoxical sleep deprivation time is more beneficial to avoid fear conditioning or posttraumatic stress memory. Also, the striatum was found to be less vulnerable to oxidative stress induced by paradoxical sleep deprivation. Compromise of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex confirmed a central role for these two structures in working and aversive memory. We suggest that the variability and lability of the observed behavior offers opportunities to manipulate unwanted consolidated memory such as in the case of posttraumatic stress.
References
Albouy G, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Darsaud A, Gais S, Rauchs G et al (2013) Interaction between hippocampal and striatal systems predicts subsequent consolidation of motor sequence memory. PLoS One 8(3):e59490PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Alzoubi KH, Khabour OF, Salah HA, Abu Rashid BE (2012) The combined effect of sleep deprivation and Western diet on spatial learning and memory: role of BDNF and oxidative stress. J Mol Neurosci 50(1):124–133PubMedCrossRef
Alzoubi KH, Khabour OF, Salah HA, Abu Rashid BE (2013) The combined effect of sleep deprivation and Western diet on spatial learning and memory: role of BDNF and oxidative stress. J Mol Neurosci 50(1):124–133PubMedCrossRef
Brown MK, Naidoo N (2010) The UPR and the anti-oxidant response: relevance to sleep and sleep loss. Mol Neurobiol 42(2):103–113PubMedCrossRef
Bruin VM, Bittencourt LR, Tufik S (2012) Sleep-wake disturbances in Parkinson’s disease: current evidence regarding diagnostic and therapeutic decisions. Eur Neurol 67(5):257–267PubMedCrossRef
Bucherelli C, Baldi E, Mariottini C, Passani MB, Blandina P (2006) Aversive memory reactivation engages in the amygdala only some neurotransmitters involved in consolidation. Learn Mem 13(4):426–430PubMedCrossRef
Burlet S, Leger L, Cespuglio R (1999) Nitric oxide and sleep in the rat: a puzzling relationship. Neuroscience 92(2):627–639PubMedCrossRef
Clement P, Sarda N, Cespuglio R, Gharib A (2004) Changes occurring in cortical NO release and brain NO-synthases during a paradoxical sleep deprivation and subsequent recovery in the rat. J Neurochem 90(4):848–856PubMedCrossRef
D’Almeida V, Lobo LL, Hipolide DC, de Oliveira AC, Nobrega JN, Tufik S (1998) Sleep deprivation induces brain region-specific decreases in glutathione levels. Neuroreport 9(12):2853–2856PubMedCrossRef
Das G, Gopalakrishnan A, Faisal M, Mallick BN (2008) Stimulatory role of calcium in rapid eye movement sleep deprivation-induced noradrenaline-mediated increase in Na-K-ATPase activity in rat brain. Neuroscience 155(1):76–89PubMedCrossRef
de Oliveira RA, Cunha GM, Borges KD, de Bruin GS, dos Santos-Filho EA, Viana GS et al (2004) The effect of venlafaxine on behaviour, body weight and striatal monoamine levels on sleep-deprived female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 79(3):499–506PubMedCrossRef
DeNoble VJ, Repetti SJ, Gelpke LW, Wood LM, Keim KL (1986) Vinpocetine: nootropic effects on scopolamine-induced and hypoxia-induced retrieval deficits of a step-through passive avoidance response in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 24(4):1123–1128PubMedCrossRef
Diekelmann S, Wilhelm I, Born J (2009) The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Med Rev 13(5):309–321PubMedCrossRef
Dzoljic E, van Leeuwen R, de Vries R, Dzoljic MR (1997) Vigilance and EEG power in rats: effects of potent inhibitors of the neuronal nitric oxide synthase. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 356(1):56–61PubMedCrossRef
Everson CA, Laatsch CD, Hogg N (2005) Antioxidant defense responses to sleep loss and sleep recovery. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 288(2):R374–R383PubMedCrossRef
Gerashchenko D, Wisor JP, Burns D, Reh RK, Shiromani PJ, Sakurai T et al (2008) Identification of a population of sleep-active cerebral cortex neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105(29):10227–10232PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Graves LA, Heller EA, Pack AI, Abel T (2003) Sleep deprivation selectively impairs memory consolidation for contextual fear conditioning. Learn Mem 10(3):168–176PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Hellman K, Hernandez P, Park A, Abel T (2010) Genetic evidence for a role for protein kinase A in the maintenance of sleep and thalamocortical oscillations. Sleep 33(1):19–28PubMedCentralPubMed
Hidaka N, Suemaru K, Takechi K, Li B, Araki H (2011) Inhibitory effects of valproate on impairment of Y-maze alternation behavior induced by repeated electroconvulsive seizures and c-Fos protein levels in rat brains. Acta Med Okayama 65(4):269–277PubMed
Kalinchuk AV, McCarley RW, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Basheer R (2010) Sleep deprivation triggers inducible nitric oxide-dependent nitric oxide production in wake-active basal forebrain neurons. J Neurosci 30(40):13254–13264PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Kapas L, Shibata M, Kimura M, Krueger JM (1994) Inhibition of nitric oxide synthesis suppresses sleep in rabbits. Am J Physiol 266(1 Pt 2):R151–R157PubMed
Khadrawy YA, Nour NA, Aboul Ezz HS (2011) Effect of oxidative stress induced by paradoxical sleep deprivation on the activities of Na+, K+-ATPase and acetylcholinesterase in the cortex and hippocampus of rat. Transl Res 157(2):100–107PubMedCrossRef
Kuriyama K, Soshi T, Kim Y (2010) Sleep deprivation facilitates extinction of implicit fear generalization and physiological response to fear. Biol Psychiatry 68(11):991–998PubMedCrossRef
Lobo V, Patil A, Phatak A, Chandra N (2010) Free radicals, antioxidants and functional foods: impact on human health. Pharmacogn Rev 4(8):118–126PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Miyata S, Noda A, Iwamoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N (2013) Poor sleep quality impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res 22(5):535–541PubMedCrossRef
Morairty SR, Dittrich L, Pasumarthi RK, Valladao D, Heiss JE, Gerashchenko D et al (2013) A role for cortical nNOS/NK1 neurons in coupling homeostatic sleep drive to EEG slow wave activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110(50):20272–20277PubMedCrossRef
Nielsen HG, Opstad PK, Lyberg T (2007) LeuCAM and reactive oxygen species during long-term exercise combined with sleep and energy deficiency. Med Sci Sports Exerc 39(2):275–282PubMedCrossRef
Patti CL, Zanin KA, Sanday L, Kameda SR, Fernandes-Santos L, Fernandes HA et al (2010) Effects of sleep deprivation on memory in mice: role of state-dependent learning. Sleep 33(12):1669–1679PubMedCentralPubMed
Ramanathan L, Siegel JM (2011) Sleep deprivation under sustained hypoxia protects against oxidative stress. Free Radic Biol Med 51(10):1842–1848PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Ramanathan L, Gulyani S, Nienhuis R, Siegel JM (2002) Sleep deprivation decreases superoxide dismutase activity in rat hippocampus and brainstem. Neuroreport 13(11):1387–1390PubMedCrossRef
Ramanathan L, Hu S, Frautschy SA, Siegel JM (2010) Short-term total sleep deprivation in the rat increases antioxidant responses in multiple brain regions without impairing spontaneous alternation behavior. Behav Brain Res 207(2):305–309PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Reimund E (1994) The free radical flux theory of sleep. Med Hypotheses 43(4):231–233PubMedCrossRef
Rodriguez-Vazquez J, Camacho-Arroyo I, Velazquez-Moctezuma J (2012) Differential impact of REM sleep deprivation on cytoskeletal proteins of brain regions involved in sleep regulation. Neuropsychobiology 65(3):161–167PubMedCrossRef
Sarter M, Bodewitz G, Stephens DN (1988) Attenuation of scopolamine-induced impairment of spontaneous alteration behaviour by antagonist but not inverse agonist and agonist beta-carbolines. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 94(4):491–495CrossRef
Shi HS, Luo YX, Xue YX, Wu P, Zhu WL, Ding ZB et al (2011) Effects of sleep deprivation on retrieval and reconsolidation of morphine reward memory in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 98(2):299–303PubMedCrossRef
Silva RH, Abilio VC, Takatsu AL, Kameda SR, Grassl C, Chehin AB et al (2004) Role of hippocampal oxidative stress in memory deficits induced by sleep deprivation in mice. Neuropharmacology 46(6):895–903PubMedCrossRef
Suer C, Dolu N, Artis AS, Sahin L, Yilmaz A, Cetin A (2011) The effects of long-term sleep deprivation on the long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus and brain oxidation status in rats. Neurosci Res 70(1):71–77PubMedCrossRef
Thatcher GR, Bennett BM, Reynolds JN (2006) NO chimeras as therapeutic agents in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 3(3):237–245PubMedCrossRef
van Hulzen ZJ, Coenen AM (1981) Paradoxical sleep deprivation and locomotor activity in rats. Physiol Behav 27(4):741–744PubMedCrossRef
Varela C, Kumar S, Yang JY, Wilson MA (2013) Anatomical substrates for direct interactions between hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the thalamic nucleus reuniens. Brain Struct Funct 4(12):1130–1133
Vazquez-Medina JP, Zenteno-Savin T, Elsner R, Ortiz RM (2012) Coping with physiological oxidative stress: a review of antioxidant strategies in seals. J Comp Physiol B 182(6):741–750PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Vollert C, Zagaar M, Hovatta I, Taneja M, Vu A, Dao A et al (2011) Exercise prevents sleep deprivation-associated anxiety-like behavior in rats: potential role of oxidative stress mechanisms. Behav Brain Res 224(2):233–240PubMedCrossRef
Wisor JP, Gerashchenko D, Kilduff TS (2011) Sleep-active neuronal nitric oxide synthase-positive cells of the cerebral cortex: a local regulator of sleep? Curr Top Med Chem 11(19):2483–2489PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
Yadav SK, Prakash J, Chouhan S, Singh SP (2013) Mucuna pruriens seed extract reduces oxidative stress in nigrostriatal tissue and improves neurobehavioral activity in paraquat-induced Parkinsonian mouse model. Neurochem Int 62(8):1039–1047PubMedCrossRef
Zhang JH, Fung SJ, Xi M, Sampogna S, Chase MH (2010) Apnea produces neuronal degeneration in the pons and medulla of guinea pigs. Neurobiol Dis 40(1):251–264PubMedCrossRef
FEEDBACK